翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Joshua Kadison
・ Joshua Kaeo
・ Joshua Kalsakau
・ Joshua Keating
・ Joshua Kekaulahao
・ Joshua Kennedy
・ Joshua Key
・ Joshua Kimmich
・ Joshua King
・ Joshua King (footballer)
・ Joshua Kiprui Cheptegei
・ Joshua Kirby
・ Joshua Kirton
・ Joshua Kissi
・ Joshua Klein
Joshua Knobe
・ Joshua Kors
・ Joshua Koshiba
・ Joshua Kosker
・ Joshua Kulp
・ Joshua Kurlantzick
・ Joshua Kushner
・ Joshua Kutuny
・ Joshua L. Bagnall
・ Joshua L. Chamberlain Museum
・ Joshua L. Foster
・ Joshua L. Goldberg
・ Joshua L. Johns
・ Joshua L. Liebman
・ Joshua L. Martin


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Joshua Knobe : ウィキペディア英語版
Joshua Knobe

Joshua Knobe is an experimental philosopher, whose work ranges across issues in philosophy of mind and action and ethics. He is an associate professor in the Program in Cognitive Science and Department of Philosophy at Yale University. He was previously Assistant Professor of Philosophy at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Knobe received his B.A. at Stanford University in 1996 and his Ph.D. from Princeton in 2006,〔See Knobe's dissertation, "Folk Psychology, Folk Morality": https://webspace.utexas.edu/deverj/personal/test/folkpsychology.pdf〕 where he studied under Gilbert Harman,〔See the list of recent philosophy dissertations at Princeton: http://philosophy.princeton.edu/recent_dissertations.html〕 among others. His work has been discussed in various media, including ''The New York Times''〔See Appiah's article "The New New Philosophy":http://www.nytimes.com/2007/12/09/magazine/09wwln-idealab-t.html〕 and ''Slate'',〔See Lackman's article "The X-Philes":http://www.slate.com/id/2137223/?nav=fo〕 and he is a fairly frequent guest on the online news, science, and current events channel bloggingheads.tv.
==The Knobe Effect==
Knobe is arguably most widely known for what has come to be called "the Knobe effect" or the "Side-Effect Effect". According to Jones (2009):
Rather than consulting his own philosophical intuitions, Knobe set out to find out how ordinary people think about intentional action. In a study published in 2003, Knobe presented passers-by in a Manhattan park with the following scenario. The CEO of a company is sitting in his office when his Vice President of R&D comes in and says, ‘We are thinking of starting a new programme. It will help us increase profits, but it will also harm the environment.’ The CEO responds that he doesn’t care about harming the environment and just wants to make as much profit as possible. The programme is carried out, profits are made and the environment is harmed.

Did the CEO intentionally harm the environment? The vast majority of people Knobe quizzed – 82 per cent – said he did. But what if the scenario is changed such that the word ‘harm’ is replaced with ‘help’? In this case the CEO doesn’t care about helping the environment, and still just wants to make a profit – and his actions result in both outcomes. Now faced with the question ‘Did the CEO intentionally help the environment?’, just 23 per cent of Knobe’s participants said ‘yes’ (Knobe, 2003a).

This asymmetry in responses between the ‘harm’ and ‘help’ scenarios, now known as the Knobe effect, provides a direct challenge to the idea of a one-way flow of judgments from the factual or non-moral domain to the moral sphere. ‘These data show that the process is actually much more complex,’ argues Knobe. Instead, the moral character of an action’s consequences also seems to influence how non-moral aspects of the action – in this case, whether someone did something intentionally or not – are judged.〔; Knobe (2003b).〕


抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Joshua Knobe」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.